By Corinna Hartmann and Sara Goudarzi, Scientific American
In spite of sharing genes and environments, siblings are
often not as similar in nature as one might think. But where do the
supposed differences come from? Alfred Adler, a 19th- and early
20th-century Austrian psychotherapist and founder of individual
psychology, suspected that birth order leads to differences in siblings.
Adler
considered firstborns to be neurotic, because they don’t have to share
their parents for years and are essentially dethroned once a sibling
comes along. He also considered oldest children dutiful and sometimes
conservative. According to Adler, the youngest children are ambitious,
while middle children are optimally positioned in the family and are
characterized by emotional stability. Adler himself was the second of
seven children.
American
psychologist Frank J. Sulloway, who, in the mid-1990s, combed history
books for leading figures who were firstborns and rebellious ones who
were born later, saw a similar trend. Among the later borns, he found
lateral thinkers and revolutionaries, such as Charles Darwin, Karl Marx
and Mahatma Gandhi. Among firstborns, he discovered leaders such as
Joseph Stalin and Benito Mussolini. His explanation? Every child
occupies a certain niche within the family and then uses his or her own
strategies to master life. Firstborn and single children had less reason
to quarrel with the status quo and identify more strongly with the
worldview of their fathers and mothers. Younger siblings are less sure
of their parents’ view and therefore more often choose alternative paths
in life.
Such
categorizations are popular because they’re rather intuitive, and one
can always find an example of the sensible big sister or the rebellious
young brother in their circle of acquaintances. As such, Adler’s words
still appear regularly in educational guides and continue to reverberate
in the minds of parents.
Furthermore, some studies confirmed the idea that sibling position can shape personality. For example, a 1968 study
showed that, compared with later borns, first borns are less likely to
participate in dangerous sports because of fears of physical injury. And
a 1980 study
of 170 female and 142 male undergraduates showed lower anxiety and
higher ego in firstborns, as measured by the Howarth Personality
Questionnaire. At times, however, these investigations used questionable
methods. For example, members of the same family were often asked to
assess themselves in terms of extraversion, openness to experiences,
conscientiousness, tolerance and neuroticism. The catch is these surveys
were conducted at only one point in time. The older siblings were
therefore not only born first but also simply older. It has long been
known that adolescents become more conscientious as they age. This trend
could account for a large part of the results. Another methodological
flaw was that only one person judged his or her own personality and that
of his or her siblings. This detail is important because
self-perception and the perception of others can sometimes differ
considerably. In addition, the test subjects may have subconsciously
incorporated the cliché of dutiful older siblings and cosmopolitan later
borns into their evaluation and could have thus brought about the
expected result themselves.
Meanwhile
scientists who analyzed large, transnational data and compared
different families with each other have found the effect of sibling
succession on personality disappears almost completely. Researchers led
by psychologist Julia Rohrer of the University of Leipzig in Germany
evaluated data
from more than 20,000 interviewees from Germany, the U.K. and the U.S.
They compared the personality profiles of siblings but also of people
with different birth orders who had never met. The Leipzig psychologists
did not discover any systematic differences in personality.
In
such studies, researchers must be particularly cautious because, in
addition to age, the size of one’s family is another factor that’s
intertwined with sibling position. A child from a family of four has a
50 percent chance of being a firstborn; the more siblings, the lower the
probability. For example, the fact that many astronauts are firstborns
does not necessarily speak to the special qualities of those born first.
It’s likely that many astronauts come from smaller families. To better
understand these influences, Rohrer and her team controlled forthe
number of siblings. That’s because when there are more of them, there
are more later borns. So the researchers hypothesized later borns may
more often appear in families of lower socioeconomic classes—which could
account for differences between children of different-sized families.
The
larger the sample, the more likely even very small effects will be
detected. For example, in a 2015 study, which included 377,000 high
school students, psychologist Rodica Damian and her colleague Brent W.
Roberts, both then at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
discovered that firstborns tended to be more conscientious, extraverted
and willing to lead. Contrary to expectations, they were also more
tolerant and emotionally stable than adolescents with older siblings.
Yet the differences were very small, and the researchers concluded that
the importance that is generally attached to sibling position in shaping
one’s character is exaggerated.
“It
is quite possible that the position in the sibling sequence shapes the
personality—but not in every family in the same way,” says Frank
Spinath, a psychologist at Saarland University in Germany. “In other
words, there may be an influence but not a systematic one. Nevertheless,
other influences weigh more heavily when it comes to the differences in
character of siblings. In addition to genes, the so-called undivided
environment also plays a role. For siblings who grow up in the same
family, this includes the respective circle of friends, for example.”
Further, parents do not treat their children the same regardless of
their birth rank. Studies show that parents react sensitively to the
innate temperament of their offspring and adapt their upbringing
accordingly.
Damian’s
study also found that on average, firstborns enjoy a small IQ advantage
over their younger siblings. Those born first also tend to complete
their education with a higher degree and opt for traditionally
prestigious careers, such as medicine or engineering.
How
does this intellectual advantage come about? Adler may be right that
the undivided attention given to the first child in early life promotes
cognitive abilities. This advantage is already apparent by the age of
two. Norwegian researchers Petter Kristensen and Tor Bjerkedal cleverly
showed that the difference in intelligence is not linked to biological
factors (some had suspected it might be related to physical conditions
during pregnancy). They tested children whose older siblings had died
early. The researchers’ assumption was that although these children were
biologically younger siblings, they assumed the role of the firstborn
in the family. Compared with other younger siblings, they achieved
better results in intelligence tests.